Authors

  1. Williamson, Selena BSN, RN
  2. McGrath, Jacqueline M. PhD, RN, FNAP, FAAN

Abstract

Background/Significance: Premature infants often experience extended stays in the neonatal intensive care unit (NICU) as opposed to home with parents. This prolonged separation creates a strain for both parents and infants, decreasing attachment and parental caregiving. One strategy to combat this shared stress is increasing parental participation, particularly through the use of their voices whether parents are present or not.

 

Purpose: This Evidence-Based Practice Brief column explores the connection between mother and child, specifically the effects of maternal voice on infant autonomic stability, weight gain, and behavioral states.

 

Methods: A systematic search of CINAHL, PubMed, and PsycInfo was used to identify studies involving the use of maternal voice intervention with preterm infants in the NICU.

 

Results: Fifteen studies were identified. Three intervention categories emerged: (1) live maternal speech, (2) recorded maternal speech (subcategories included whether intervention content was prescribed or not), and (3) recorded maternal speech that was combined with biological maternal sounds (heart rate). Within each category, studies were organized chronologically to reflect how knowledge has changed overtime.

 

Implications for Practice: Maternal voice has physiological as well as behavioral and emotional effect on preterm infants. Several studies found that maternal voice increased autonomic stability improving (heart rate and respirations) as well as weight gain. No negative effects were identified. Given these findings, incorporating different types of maternal voice into routine care by the bedside nurse can assist the mother in feeling more involved in her infant's care without seemingly being a distraction or obstacle to providers.

 

Implications for Research: A major limitation for generalizability was sample size; more research is needed with larger sample sizes replicating interventions types to discern best outcomes.

 

Video Abstract available at https://journals.lww.com/advancesinneonatalcare/Pages/videogallery.aspx?videoId=.

 

Article Content

The early attachment between the mother and the child is one that is recognized by many for its importance to fetal and infant development. While the development of the attachment in utero is vital, that connection does not stop once the neonate is born. In fact, one could argue that the importance of the connection increases as the neonate continues to develop, particularly in relationship to the language, visual, and cognitive development.1 For neonates born premature or high-risk, admission to the neonatal intensive care unit (NICU) is likely. Within this chaotic and abnormal environment, these high-risk infants are challenged to develop normally without constant contact with their parents, particularly their mother, as their primary caregiver. Not only does this separation place more stress on the already physically stressed infant, but it also adds stress for the mother, increasing her feelings of helplessness, particularly because she cannot fully participate in her child's caregiving and development in the way she truly yearns.2 Mother-infant attachment is well-known to be related to maternal proximity, touch, and voice and each of these characteristics provides support to the infant.3 In the NICU environment, this attachment is often challenged to fully develop because of obstacles in the environment including limited mother-infant exposure, direct care provided by a nonparent, and other physical limitations (such as being cared for in an incubator or limitations on physical contact between the mother and the child).4

 

To address this issue, much research has occurred related to how to better incorporate mothers into their premature infants' care within the NICU setting; unfortunately, not much research has been completed examining ways to increase father participation. Parents often struggle with coordinating their availability between their personal lives and the predetermined visiting hours set by the hospital, even with open visitation many NICUs still have times when parents cannot visit, such as during nursing report or medical rounds. Many parents must continue working and potentially care for other children, so they are unable to be with their newborn infant as often as they would prefer. Given these circumstances, many researchers have examined the feasibility of audio recordings as a source of parental interaction. Two early studies by Lahav and colleagues5,6 examined the feasibility and potential safety and care issues related to this type of therapy. These feasibility studies showed that audio recordings of voice as well as biological maternal sounds (BMS) were safe to be used in the NICU setting. However, in the preliminary research, the therapeutic window has not been determined, nor the optimal range for this type of therapy, or the short- and long-term effects, both behavioral and physical, that these therapies could have for infants and their mothers.

 

It is widely accepted that a mother's voice is soothing to her infant, but to what extent or capacity this effect has on the physiological and behavioral responses in the very preterm and moderate to late preterm infant is not well understood. The short- and long-term effects of maternal voice on physiological and behavioral responses were not addressed in these beginning feasibility studies. Increasing our understanding of the connection between mother and preterm infant from a scientific vantage point provides more evidence to support the integration of this intervention into routine caregiving in the NICU. Thus, the purpose of this systematic review is to gain an understanding of what is currently known about the concept of maternal voice as a therapeutic agent for the preterm infant in the NICU.

 

PURPOSE

This systematic review explores the connection between mother and child, specifically the effects of maternal voice on infant autonomic stability, weight gain, and behavioral states. Given the varied types of interventions, we were unable to complete a meta-analysis; however, we did complete a systematic search and synthesis of the literature using PRISMA to guide our work. A cursory literature search illustrated that the primary source of parental auditory stimulation came from the mother, with little research being conducted with fathers. Why this occurred is unknown, although it may be related to known research on the importance of the mother-infant attachment. Thus, for the review only studies of maternal voice are included.

 

METHODS

Inclusion and exclusion criteria for study identification are outlined in Table 1. Studies must have met all of the criteria to be included. Studies were identified through a systematic search of CINAHL, PubMed, and its related database MEDLINE, and PsycInfo using the search words as identified in Table 2. Studies were also found through ScienceDirect and Google Scholar.

  
Table 1 - Click to enlarge in new windowTABLE 1. Study Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria
 
Table 2 - Click to enlarge in new windowTABLE 2. Search Terms Used for Systemically Searching the Literature (PubMed)

Cochrane Library was included in our search but not utilized since no reports were found addressing this topic. The initial searches were completed in March 2017 and repeated in April 2018. For specific databases, more intense parameters were included such as the use of MESH term. A PRISMA diagram (Figure 1) was constructed to illustrate this process. After deleting duplicates, a total of 72 articles were found via the database search. With completion of an initial screening, 43 articles were discarded for not meeting parameters: not being available in English; not written in the time frame of the past 15 years; and not occurring within the NICU. Articles were further screened for eligibility regarding the material, such as an intervention study using maternal voice as a therapeutic agent and focusing on preterm infants. Studies were evaluated and grouped on the basis of eligibility and study focus. A total of 15 intervention studies are included in the review. These studies were read in detail and categorized by theme on the basis of maternal voice intervention type into (1) live Maternal Speech or Singing; (2) Recorded Speech or Singing; and then (3) Recorded BMS with Maternal Speech. These categories were further grouped into subsets on the basis of whether the content the mothers spoke and/or sang was prescribed in the study protocol.

  
Figure 1 - Click to enlarge in new windowFIGURE 1. PRISMA diagram: Maternal voice effects with Preterm Infants. A PRISMA flow diagram illustrating the screening and organization process used when constructing this review.

Each reviewer read the final 15 studies and came to consensus to design the matrix table (Table 3). Agreement for inclusion/exclusion of a study in the review was 95%; the only areas where consensus building occurred were (1) whether the study involved predominantly preterm or full-term infants and (2) whether the study was predominately a maternal voice intervention.

  
Table 3 - Click to enlarge in new windowTABLE 3. Matrix Table: Maternal Voice Effects on Preterm Infants: A Matrix Table Illustrating the Highlights of Each Study Included in This Reviewa

Once studies were identified, information from studies was extracted and compiled into a matrix table. The table had the following categories: author, year, title, journal, type of study; purpose (research question, hypotheses); methods (setting/sample design); study variables (independent, dependent); measures (reliability/validity, instruments, data collection methods); results; limitations summary (decision/reservations). Each study was also examined for level of evidence such that comparisons could be made (see Matrix Table 3). The matrix table served as a summary of the studies and aided in further organization of the studies based on similarities and differences in the study content, design, and results (see Table 3).

 

RESULTS

Of the 15 studies included in the review, 3 involved live speech/singing, 9 with recorded speech/singing, and 3 others with recorded biological maternal sounds and speech. Biological maternal sounds refer to the recording of the mother's heartbeat combined with the mother's vocal recording. Of the 9 studies that used recorded maternal speech/singing, 2 of the studies did not prescribe the intervention content and 7 did (meaning that the participants [mothers] were required to follow a script). Recordings were played for the infant using mp3 players and similar technology. For the studies with live singing and BMS, the mothers could choose the content of their interaction with their infants. Study designs varied and included single subject, within subject, descriptive, quasi-experimental, prospective clinical trial, and longitudinal case study (see Table 3 for more details). Most included studies were conducted within relatively short time frames, a few days to a few weeks, although there are instances of elongated review, such as in one longitudinal study.

 

Independent variables for studies predominately focused on presence or absence of maternal voice, whether it be live or recorded. Some studies also compared the control (no voice) to maternal voice and additional auditory stimulation, such as music or alternative voice, such as the nurses. Dependent variables extracted from the studies were also similar. Several studies examined infant physiological effects through assessment of heart rate, oxygen saturation, and respiration rate, which we define as physical effects. Other studies examined biological measures such as salivary cortisol levels to determine the biological effects maternal voice had on the preterm infant. Furthermore, some studies also examined feeding habits, such as volume of formula consumed, days until full oral feedings obtained, and weight gain to address an outcome of biological effects. Additional studies also examined behavioral responses, through the use of behavioral observation instruments such as the Blackburn Activity Scale, which we define as behavioral effects. The studies included in this review are discussed within these 3 categories to assist clinicians in considering which types of interventions might be most relevant in their caregiving situation. We have also included more detail about the interventions provided in our discussion of study results since the study designs and interventions vary greatly and the detail could be helpful to those considering incorporating these interventions into routine care (Table 4).

  
Table 4 - Click to enlarge in new windowTABLE 4. Evidence-Based Recommendations for Use of Maternal Voice in NICU

Live Maternal Speech or Singing

This independent variable included in this category of studies was live maternal speech or singing. Blumenfeld and Eisenfeld8 were the first to investigate the potential therapeutic effects of live maternal voice with preterm infants. They examined the effects of mothers singing to their preterm infants during feedings and measured the infant's heart rate, respiration rate, and volume of formula taken orally (n = 11 mother-neonate pairs). Neonates served as their own controls and no significant differences were identified in relationship to the intervention. Although behavior measures were not obtained, the researchers noted that the neonates were more relaxed with the intervention. Filippa and colleagues (2013) used a single-subject design with 18 mothers and their infants2; this study differed from Blumenfeld and Eisenfeld's in that the singing/speech condition did not occur during feedings. The sing/speech condition was delivered by mothers on an every-other day basis; on odd days control data were collected. Results showed that critical respiratory events were significantly lower in the maternal voice condition, and that heart rate and oxygen saturation were significantly higher. Despite having a relatively small sample size, their results illustrated both positive physical and behavioral/emotional effects of maternal voice with the preterm infant, demonstrating its potential use as a therapeutic agent. Similarly, Arnon and associates3 used a randomized within-subject crossover design with 86 preterm infants born between 32 and 36 weeks' gestation. Mothers sang to their infants during skin-to-skin care. Measurements included infant's state, infant heart rate variability, as well as physiological parameters (respiratory rate, heart rate, and oxygen saturation) for both mothers and infants and maternal anxiety. Results demonstrated increased autonomic and physiological stability for the infant regarding heart rate variability with the intervention. Maternal anxiety was also decreased. This was one of the few studies to examine both infants and maternal outcomes. This study also had the largest sample size by far among all the included studies.

 

Recordings of Maternal Voice (Uncontrolled Content)

Two groups of researchers used maternal voice recordings (no control of content during recordings) to perform their trials. In 2007, Johnston and colleagues9 completed a crossover design study, using the mother's voice as a distracting and soothing element during heel lance procedures. The intervention included maternal voice recordings provided after feedings 3 times daily over a 2-day period, and then the same recordings were played during the heel-lance procedure. Ten days after this first trial, the trial was repeated; however, the recordings were not played during this second heel lance to examine differences related to the use of the recordings during the procedure. Results were inconclusive for most variables. However, infants exposed to the maternal sounds gained significantly more weight than their matched controls who did not receive the intervention. In the second study, Sajjadian and associates10 used a single-subject design, where the infants served as their own controls. Twenty neonates received the intervention every day for 3 days, 3 times a day. The intervention consisted of three 5-minute intervals: the first 5 minutes were the control period, where no sound was played, followed by a 5-minute interval of the mother's recording, and followed by another 5-minute interval of silence again. Neonates' heart rate, respiration rate, and oxygen saturation rate were recorded during these sessions. Neonates' oxygen saturation increased during the intervention as compared with the control conditions, while heart rate and respiration rate decreased, illustrating autonomic stabilization. No long-term outcomes were measured.

 

Recordings of Maternal Voice (Controlled Content)

This group of studies used recordings of the mothers' voices and further controlled the content of the speech during the intervention. Study protocols had the mothers speak the same phrase, or choose from a list of a selected nursery rhymes, read from a passage from a book, or sing the same songs. Surprisingly, many of these studies choose the same book, The Little Prince by Antoine de Saint-Exupery. There was no explanation for choosing this book particularly, however it may, because of the novel's rhythmic pace and popularity among parents at the time.

 

Saito et al4 used a single-subject design with 26 preterm infants to examine infants' responses when listening to their mothers' voice as compared with listening to a nurse's voice. Testing consisted of playing the recording of the same phrases by either the nurse or the mother for 10 seconds and followed by white noise (control) for 20 seconds; this sequence was repeated 2 more times to complete a "block." These blocks were repeated 4 times, so that the total amount of time of intervention sessions was 10 minutes. Each infant completed a session with the mother's recording and the nurse's recording; infants randomly received either the mother's or the nurse's recording first in the protocol sequence. Near-infrared technology was used to examine changes in infants' oxygenated hemoglobin, deoxygenated hemoglobin, and total hemoglobin in blood from the brain frontal area to determine where activation of certain lobes occurred across the intervention sequence. Different areas of the brain were activated; during the nurse's recording, both the infant's right and left frontal lobes were activated, whereas during the mother's recording, only the infant's left frontal lobe was activated, and the activity level was increased as compared with the nurse's recording. These results demonstrated the infant's ability to differentiate between voices they hear and potentially identify their mother's voices from another female's voice. The researchers suggest that during the intervention the infants' brains made a connection with the nurse's voice and pain, as this was the voice they heard during procedures and care, whereas the area activated more by the mother's voice was connected to more positive emotions and could increase emotional attachment. However, these outcomes are speculative; more research is needed to better understand these relationships.

 

Bozzette11 recruited 14 preterm infants to participate in a quasi-experimental study. Mothers were each asked to record themselves for 3 minutes reading the same script, a nursery rhyme. The experimental sessions consisted of 3 minutes of baseline conditions, 3 minutes of the mother's voice recording, followed by another 3 minutes to return to baseline. Recording was played 4 times a day for 3 days. Infants' physiological responses-heart rate, respiration rate, and oxygen saturation as well as the infants' behavioral responses-were measured throughout the protocol. Results showed that infants' respiration rates decreased, stability behaviors (foot clasp, handclasp, sigh, ooh face, open face, flexion, grasping, and smile) were increased as well as attending behavior (eyes wide, eyes brightening, eyes toward source, suckling, stilling, visual locking, hand to face/mouth) was increased during the maternal recordings. Similarly in 2010, Krueger and colleagues12 conducted a comparative quasi-experimental study with 54 preterm infants.12 Infants were divided into 3 groups: group 1 (n = 16) received a recording of their mother's voice reciting nursery rhymes twice a day from week 28 to 34 PMA (postmenstrual age); group 2 (n = 17) received a recording of their mother reciting nursery rhymes twice a day during weeks 32 to 34 PMA; group 3 (n = 20) served as a control and did not receive any recording. Recordings were played for a total of 45 seconds each session. Short-term outcomes measures included the number of days to discharge, average daily weight gain, the number of days to full enteral feedings, number of days to full oral feeding, the number of episodes of feeding intolerance, days NPO, and percentage of days on respiratory support. Group 1 experienced fewer episodes of feeding intolerance in comparison with group 2 and the control group. These results suggest that the earlier use of maternal voice as a therapeutic intervention may aid in increasing the rate at which preterm infants become tolerant to oral feedings and move from enteral feedings to oral feedings, which aids in increased weight velocity.

 

In 2014, Dorn and colleagues13 conducted a prospective clinical trial with block randomization (n = 62 neonates).13 Neonates were split into multiple groups: group 1 (n = 20) received a recording of music, group 2 (n = 20) received a recording of their mother reciting passages from The Little Prince, and group 3 (n = 22) was a matched control group of infants who received no recordings. Recordings were played during a specific time period for 30 minutes every day for 2 weeks. Salivary cortisol levels were measured 10 minutes before recordings and 10 minutes after the recordings were played. As a baseline, salivary cortisol levels were also collected 7 times over a 24-hour period before initiating the intervention. Study results were inconclusive; no significant differences were noted in salivary cortisol levels across the measurement events; no other parameters were measured during this study. Picciolini et al14 conducted a trial that utilized recordings of maternal readings of The Little Prince as well. Within this longitudinal explorative case study, 71 preterm infants were compared with matched controls. Voice recordings were played via bone conductors to mimic the vibrations the infant felt while in utero when their mother spoke. The recordings were played 3 times daily for 3 weeks. The infants were observed during testing, and at 3-month corrected age, and 6-month corrected age. Measurements included vital and neurobehavioral parameters at term, neurofunctional assessment at 3 and 6 months of corrected age, heart rate, oxygen saturation, presence of tremors, changes in skin color, and quality of spontaneous motor activity. At each study point, the group that received the maternal voice recordings had lower heart rates and more stable skin color as well as higher scores for visual attention and quality of movements as compared with the matched control group. At 3 months, the intervention group had higher scores for the neurofunctional assessment; however, by month 6 the 2 groups performed similarly in all the tests. This study was interesting because it was the only one to use a vibratory element in their delivery of the maternal voice to the infant, attempting to mimic the type of environment the infant first heard his or her mother's voice. It also was one of the only studies to examine the therapeutic effects for a long term. In 2016, Wirth and colleagues15 conducted a randomized, controlled nonblind study with 62 infants. Twenty infants received recordings of classical lullabies, 20 others received recordings of their mother's voice reading from The Little Prince, and 22 infants were in the control group. The recordings were played for 30 minutes daily for a 2-week period, between 8:00 and 9:00 in the evening, 30 to 60 minutes after a feeding and parental contact. Measurements included examining differences in mean heart rate during stimulation compared with baseline; difference in mean heart rate before and after stimulation; difference in mean respiratory rate before, during, and after stimulation; and physical activity of the infant during the stimulation. Both groups that received the recordings (lullaby and maternal recordings) had a lower heart and respiratory rate than the control group. Physical activity was lowest in the maternal voice recording group, followed by the lullaby group, and then the control group.

 

In 2014, Chorna and associates16 conducted a randomized trial with 94 preterm infants, examining whether maternal voice would improve infant feeding behaviors. Forty-six were in the experimental group, while 48 were in the control group. The experimental group received a pacifier-activated music (PAM) player equipped with a recording of the infant's mother's voice; the recording was automatically played once a sucking threshold was reached via the pacifier. Recordings consisted of the mothers singing 2 preselected songs. The intervention period occurred for 15 minutes for 5 days, 30 TO 45 minutes before scheduled care and feedings. Baseline data for the experimental group were collected the previous 72 hours before the experimental protocol started. Measurements included the infant's oral feeding rate, the number of oral feedings, and oral feed volume consumed. Infants who received the PAM intervention had an increased oral feeding rate, volume of oral intake, as well as the number of oral feedings per day when compared to the control group. The experimental group reached full oral feedings an average of 7 days before their control group counterparts. This difference could be credited to the PAM player; if the infant sucked harder and more frequently on the pacifier, they heard their mother's voice possibly providing more incentive (the soothing voice of their mother) to practice their sucking motions and allowed them to master this skill quicker, allowing for the results of quicker oral feeding rates. More research is needed to better understand this mechanism.

 

Recordings of Maternal Voice and Biological Maternal Sounds

These studies examined the relationship between maternal voice, biological sounds, and infant responses. In 2012, Doheny and associates17 conducted their study with 14 preterm infants where infants served as their own controls. Infants were played a recording of their mother reading and singing as well as the mother's heartbeat; the sounds were filtered together to mimic how they would sound in the womb. The recordings were played for 30 minutes 4 times a day, beginning a week after the infant's birth until the infant was discharged from the NICU. Thus, some infants received the intervention for a longer duration than others. The control group did not receive any recordings. Measurements included the number of critical respiratory events (CREs) each infant experienced. Results showed that the infants receiving the intervention experienced a lower number of CREs than their control counterparts. It was also determined that the intervention proved most significant (most difference in frequency of CREs) for infants during weeks 33 to 36.6 PMA, demonstrating a potential window for therapeutic effectiveness for the use of maternal voice.

 

Similarly in 2013, Zimmerman et al18 conducted a study with a matched control design with 32 preterm infants. Sixteen infants in the experimental group received recordings of their mother's speech and heartbeat, while the control group (16 infants) received standard care. The recordings were played for 45 minutes 4 times a day for 28 days. Measurements included the infant's growth velocity (the days to regain their predicted birth weight had they been born full-term), duration of no feeding by mouth, days until full enteral feedings (defined as 140 mL/kg/day), caloric intake, and total fluid intake. Results showed that infants who received the BMS gained weight significantly faster than controls over time. Lastly in 2014, Rand and Lahav19 completed a single-subject design trial with 20 preterm infants. The infants' heart rate was measured during 30-minute increments of exposure to BMS (similar to those provided in the previous study) 4 times a day, between caregiving and parental visits, compared with nonexposure events twice each week during the infant's first month of life. The researchers found that the infants' heart rates during the exposure periods were lower than those during the nonexposure periods.

 

DISCUSSION

The main findings of these studies support the use of maternal voice as a therapeutic agent in the preterm infant's care within the NICU. Overall, the studies showed that autonomic stability improved in the presence of maternal voice (form did not seem to matter) and increased weight velocity was also noted. The use of maternal voice did not appear to produce any negative effects of outcomes for the preterm infants; even in the cases where the interventions did not produce significant results, no harm or increased stress was noted in study findings. These are important and significant effects, as many infants born premature are severely underweight and have major difficulties controlling their heart and respiration rates (autonomic control), as these systems are often immature and underdeveloped.

 

Also, important to note is the reduction of maternal anxiety and stress regarding caregiving within the NICU due to the intervention. Often, mothers feel unable to truly parent in the open environments of many NICUs for fear of judgment. Some parents have reported "stage fright"; not many individuals would feel comfortable talking, let alone singing in an environment surrounded by so many others. The recordings serve as a parenting participation strategy that the mothers can control, reducing not only their anxiety but also reinforcing their attachment to their child and alleviating their fear or discomfort of not being able to more fully participate in the care of their child. Yet, it is also important to note that none of the researchers reported attempts to control the external environment stimuli as an aspect of their study protocols that must be considered a limitation of their findings. Not having information on the external environment or other noise exposure limits recommendations for use. Within the NICU, there are many noxious stimuli, such as harsh lights, sounds, and various people talking and performing tasks that could add to the stress experienced by both infants and their mothers.

 

IMPLICATIONS FOR PRACTICE

Based on these findings, several different strategies using maternal voice recordings could be easily implemented into routine nursing care with preterm infants. The primary recommendation for care providers would be to play recordings of maternal voice during times when the mother and the father are unable to visit.9 Finding times to use maternal voice interventions to augment care could be beneficial to the infant and maximize the effects of the maternal voice while in the NICU. These recordings could be played during feedings as well as during nursing care; however, care must be taken to modulate the external environment and observe the infant's cues and behaviors so that the stimulation does not add stress for the infant.9 This may mean modulating the volume of the recordings or more likely modulating the other noise and chaos in the environment. Other recommendations, given their feasibility in individual units and environments, might include (1) linking the recordings to PAM players,16 (2) filtering recordings through a program to better simulate the womb-like environment to be more like what the infant originally heard his or her mother's voice through,14 (3) as well as linking the vocal recording to a recording of the mother's biological sounds, such as her heartbeat.19 The infant PMA must also be considered when choosing the intervention for the infant, adding maternal womb sounds to the maternal voice intervention may not be appropriate for all infants particularly those of a later PMA and when they have been experiencing the external environment for a longer period of time.

 

Linking maternal voice to feedings has been shown to increase weight gain in preterm infants,16 whereas linking maternal voice to caregiving has led to overall autonomic stability in preterm infants' heart rate, respirations, as well as a reduction of respiratory events.14,19 However, all interventions that increase stimulation for the preterm infant must be administered with attention to individual responses of the infant. Care must be taken to make sure that the stimulation does not add stress for an infant who is already easily overstimulated.

 

For mothers, the primary recommendations would likely be related to the content of the recordings. Mothers are advised to speak in soothing tones as if they were speaking to their child directly, a tone dubbed by professionals as "motherese" or "mother speak" as most appropriate.4 While there is no clear consensus over whether reading from a script or ad-libbing is more beneficial, the tone can be better controlled if the mother is reading from a book or lullaby.15 What's most important is maintaining the tone throughout the recording, as it is this maternal tone rather than that of any female that is more beneficial for the infant.12 If feasible, connecting the recording to BMS17 or skin to skin holding (kangaroo care)3 has proven to be most effective in aiding the infant in maintaining autonomic stability and weight gain. Last, there are some beginning recommendations for utilizing these recordings during procedures. Maternal recordings can be played to the infant before beginning the procedure9; while there has not been research completed on what the optimal amount of time is, a recommendation can be made for 10 to 15 minutes before the procedure, as this will allow proper time for the infant's heart rate and respiration rate to stabilize.9 If feasible, the maternal voice recordings can be played during the procedure itself, as the infant would still be able to hear whether awake or under anesthesia.9 Last, the recordings could be played directly after and during the healing process for the infant.9 Using maternal voice during procedures is an area where more research is needed. However, understanding how best to interpret the infant's cues and behaviors during the intervention will be important to guiding both short- and long-term outcomes. Additional stress must always be considered when adding intervention strategies since the other stressors in the environment must be considered and modulated or eliminated whenever possible.

 

LIMITATIONS

Several limitations must be considered. Generalization can only be made with great caution because the studied intervention protocols vary greatly across studies and most of the sample sizes were quite small. Not only is the population itself small (preterm infant in the NICU), but research is difficult to conduct with this high-risk population. Studying high-risk infants in the NICU is limited by with many challenges including (1) many healthcare needs limiting stability for research protocols, (2) limited time to perform study protocols, and importantly (3) limited access due to parental fear of research. Parents are reluctant to include their children in research protocols for fear that they will add to the stress their infants already encounter in the NICU. Several of the researchers reported maternal discomfort with participating (performing) in the study themselves, because speech and/or singing is such a vulnerable action, let alone allowing their children who often have more complex health needs to participate in such studies. These types of studies can also be time-consuming; some parents are not able to dedicate the required time for involvement. Several of the studies reported difficulty in retaining subjects, some participants dropped out due to anxiety and other factors, some infants were discharged home prior to study completion, some infants were transferred to other units/hospitals, and unfortunately some infants died. These sampling issues limit generalizability of the results.

 

IMPLICATIONS FOR FUTURE RESEARCH

Future research might examine the therapeutic window surrounding maternal voice recordings used as an intervention strategy with preterm infants. Questions could include determining if there is a particular time of day or particular point in time within the infant's caregiving schedule that proves to be more therapeutic. For example, delivering the intervention with feeding or during particular caregiving activities might increase intervention effectiveness. Also, more research is needed to better understand whether mimicking the environment of the uterus during recordings is truly significant or should only be considered for a particular population of infants (possibly those born extremely premature). Perhaps these types of recordings could be more therapeutic during stressful procedures; however, no research exists on the use of this intervention in this way, with this population. A study in progress is examining the potential effects of maternal voice on sleep patterns; although preliminary results look exciting, final results are unavailable at the time of this review.20

 

Further research could also examine whether there are any marked differences between maternal and paternal voices as a therapeutic intervention. Only one study was found using paternal voice as an intervention with preterm infants. Lee and White-Traut21 examined the effects of male recorded lullabies on preterm infants in the NICU and found the intervention reduced heart rate and increased blood oxygen saturation. While their study was aimed at introducing music into evidence-based practice, it brings up an interesting question. This type of intervention could be a positive way to incorporate the father into caregiving by providing his voice recordings in addition to the mother's. This would allow for even greater parental involvement in the premature infant's care, not only potentially improving the child's care, but also improving the relationship between father and child and the father's emotional status regarding his involvement in his child's care, a relationship that is not as stressed in our society.

 

In conclusion, promoting the use of "live" mother's voice is always the best strategy for supporting the infant. Helping the mother to understand her infant's cues and speak in "motherese" to her infant on a regular basis when she is with her infant is important to mother-infant attachment and other long-term outcomes. Nurses need to find ways to help the mother to feel more comfortable in providing this support to her infant in the environment of the NICU, since the hospital environment can be an obstacle for delivering this interaction. Provision of single rooms can help but having the available environment is just not enough. Mothers need to feel supported and believe they will not be judged on the basis of their performance (how they speak to their infant); few of us would want to perform parenting acts in the "bubble" of the NICU environment. Moreover, when it is not feasible for the mother to provide "live" speech and recorded speech and/or singing can be beneficial for infant outcomes, particularly the effects related to autonomic stability and supporting weight gain.

  
Summary of Recommend... - Click to enlarge in new windowSummary of Recommendations for Practice and Research

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

This integrative review was funded by the Social Sciences, Humanities, and Arts Research Experience (SHARE) Award from the University of Connecticut, Office of Undergraduate Research. Funding for this review was provided by the University of Connecticut's Office of Undergraduate Research

 

References

 

1. Stolt S, Korja R, Matomaki J, Lapinleimu H, Haataja L, Lehtonen L. Early relations between language development and the quality of mother-child interaction in very-low-birth-weight children. Early Hum Dev. 2014;90(5):219-225. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2014.02.007. [Context Link]

 

2. Filippa M, Devouche E, Arioni C, Imberty M, Gratier M. Live maternal speech and singing have beneficial effects on hospitalized preterm infants. Acta Paediatr. 2013;102(10):1017-1020. doi:10.1111/apa.12356. [Context Link]

 

3. Arnon S, Diamant C, Bauer S, Regev R, Sirota G, Litmanovitz I. Maternal singing during kangaroo care led to autonomic stability in preterm infants and reduced maternal anxiety. Acta Paediatr. 2014;103(10):1039-1044. doi:10.1111/apa.12744 [Context Link]

 

4. Saito Y, Fukuhara R, Aoyama S, Toshima T. Frontal brain activation in premature infants response to auditory stimuli in neonatal intensive care unit. Early Hum Dev. 2009;85(7):471-474. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2009.04.004. [Context Link]

 

5. Panagiotidis J, Lahav A. Simulation of prenatal maternal sounds in NICU incubators: a pilot safety and feasibility study. J Matern Fetal Neonatal Med. 2010;23(suppl 3):106-109. doi:10.3109/14767058.2010.512185. [Context Link]

 

6. Zimmerman E, McMahon E, Doheny L, Levine P, Lahav A. Transmission of biological maternal sounds does not interfere with routine NICU care: assessment of dose variability in very low birth weight infants [published online ahead of print October 2012]. J Pediatr Neonat Individual Med. doi:10.7363/010107. [Context Link]

 

7. Melnyk BM, Fineout-Overholt E. Box 1.3: Rating system for the hierarchy of evidence for intervention/treatment questions. In: Evidence-Based Practice in Nursing & Healthcare: A Guide to Best Practice. 3rd ed. Philadelphia, PA: Wolters Kluwer Health; 2015:11.

 

8. Blumenfeld H, Eisenfeld L. Does a mother singing to her premature baby affect feeding in the neonatal intensive care unit?Clin Pediatr. 2006;45(1):65-70. doi:10.1177/000992280604500110. [Context Link]

 

9. Johnston CC, Filion F, Nuyt AM. Recorded maternal voice for preterm neonates undergoing heel lance. Adv Neonatal Care. 2007;7(5):258-266. doi:10.1097/01.anc.0000296634.26669.13. [Context Link]

 

10. Sajjadian N, Mohammadzadeh M, Taheri PA, Shariat M. Positive effects of low intensity recorded maternal voice on physiologic reactions in premature infants. Infant Behav Dev. 2017;46:59-66. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2016.11.009. [Context Link]

 

11. Bozzette M. Healthy preterm infant responses to taped maternal voice. J Perinat Neonatal Nurs. 2008;22(4):317-318. doi:10.1097/01.jpn.0000341363.83563.ce. [Context Link]

 

12. Krueger C, Parker L, Chiu S-H, Theriaque D. Maternal voice and short-term outcomes in preterm infants. Dev Psychobiol. 2010;52(2):205-212. doi:10.1002/dev.20426. [Context Link]

 

13. Dorn F, Wirth L, Gorbey S, et al Influence of acoustic stimulation on the circadian and ultradian rhythm of premature infants. Chronobiol Int. 2014;31(9):1062-1074. doi:10.3109/07420528.2014.948183. [Context Link]

 

14. Picciolini O, Porro M, Meazza A, et al Early exposure to maternal voice: effects on preterm infants development. Early Hum Dev. 2014;90(6):287-292. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2014.03.003. [Context Link]

 

15. Wirth L, Dorn F, Zemlin M, et al Effects of standardized acoustic stimulation in premature infants: a randomized controlled trial. J Perinatol. 2016;36(6):486-492. doi:10.1038/jp.2016.1. [Context Link]

 

16. Chorna O, Slaughter J, Wang L, Stark A, Maitre N. A pacifier-activated music player with mother's voice improves oral feeding in preterm infants. Pediatrics. 2014;133(3):462-468. doi:10.1542/peds.2013-2547d. [Context Link]

 

17. Doheny L, Hurwitz S, Insoft R, Ringer S, Lahav A. Exposure to biological maternal sounds improves cardiorespiratory regulation in extremely preterm infants. J Matern Fetal Neonatal Med. 2012;25(9):1591-1594. doi:10.3109/14767058.2011.648237. [Context Link]

 

18. Zimmerman E, Keunen K, Norton M, Lahav A. Weight gain velocity in very low-birth-weight infants: effects of exposure to biological maternal sounds. Am J Perinatol. 2013;30(10):863-870. doi:10.1055/s-0033-1333669. [Context Link]

 

19. Rand K, Lahav A. Maternal sounds elicit lower heart rate in preterm newborns in the first month of life. Early Hum Dev. 2014;90(10):679-683. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2014.07.016. [Context Link]

 

20. Shellhaas RA, Barks JD, Burns JW, Hassan F, Chervin RD. Impact of maternal voice on sleep of neonates in the intensive care unit. Sleep. 2018;41:A302. (suppl 1). doi:10.1093/sleep/zsy061.813. [Context Link]

 

21. Lee H, White-Traut R. Physiologic responses of preterm infants to the male and female voice in the NICU. J Pediatr Nurs. 2014;29(1):e3-e5. doi:10.1016/j.pedn.2013.04.007. [Context Link]

 

For more than 127 additional continuing education articles related to neonatal topics, go to http://NursingCenter.com/CE.

 

biological maternal sounds; live speech; maternal singing; maternal speech; maternal voice; NICU; premature infant; preterm infant; preterm infant outcomes; recorded speech